Jump to content

Suffixaufnahme

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Suffixaufnahme (German: [ˈzʊfɪksˌaʊfˌnaːmə], "suffix resumption"), also known as case stacking, is a linguistic phenomenon used in forming a genitive construction, whereby prototypically a genitive noun agrees with its head noun. The term Suffixaufnahme itself is literally translated as "taking up of suffixes", which can be interpreted as the identical case marking of different but referentially-related phrases, with the presumption that nominal phrases possess a flat or non-configurational syntax.[1] Across syntactic theories, case is seen as a bundle of features, and case agreement as the identity of case features.[2] It was first recognized in Old Georgian and some other Caucasian and ancient Middle Eastern languages as well as many Australian languages, and almost invariably coincides with agglutinativity.

The usage of case stacking is not limited to genitive constructions, although the genitive case is involved in the majority of occurrences across languages. Cross-linguistic variations in case stacking representation and functions can be found. In general, case-stacking describes the phenomenon whereby a single word may bear multiple cases reflecting its relation to a number of different syntactic elements. It is important in the development of theories of inflectional morphology and the establishment of the relation between morphology and syntax.[3]

Examples of case-stacking

[edit]

Kayardild

[edit]

In Kayardild, the combination of adnominal cases (ablative and genitive) in possessor-like functions, and various relational and adverbial (e.g., spatial) cases is common.[4] Case functions may be combined where ablative functions similar to a genitive adnominal case, and the instrumental case is applied to the entire NP, as seen in the following example :

dan-kinaba-nguni

this-ABL-INS

dangka-naba-nguni

man-ABL-INS

mirra-nguni

good-INS

walbu-nguni

raft-INS

dan-kinaba-nguni dangka-naba-nguni mirra-nguni walbu-nguni

this-ABL-INS man-ABL-INS good-INS raft-INS

'with this man's good raft' (Evans 1995: 105)[5]

From this example, all words of the relevant phrase share the case marking which is assigned to the phrase as a whole, namely, the instrumental case. This follows Evans's (1995) total concord principle, which states that case inflections are distributed over all sub-constituents regardless of the level they are originated from (i.e., the head or its dependents).[5]

A similar example can be illustrated:

Maku-ntha

woman-OBL

yalawu-jarra-ntha

catch-PAST-OBL

yakuri-naa-ntha

fish-MABL-OBL

dangka-karra-nguni-naa-ntha

man-GEN-INST-MABL-OBL

mijil-nguni-naa-ntha

net-INST-MABL-OBL

Maku-ntha yalawu-jarra-ntha yakuri-naa-ntha dangka-karra-nguni-naa-ntha mijil-nguni-naa-ntha

woman-OBL catch-PAST-OBL fish-MABL-OBL man-GEN-INST-MABL-OBL net-INST-MABL-OBL

'The woman must have caught fish with the man's net.' (Kracht 2003: 37)

In this example, every item is marked by the oblique case (OBL), which indicates the non–indicative mood. The object is marked with the modal ablative case (MABL) and the instrumental adjunct by the instrumental (INST), and the possessor phrase by an additional genitive (GEN) as well.[6]

In addition, Kayardild also has a system of modal (i.e., aspect-mood) cases, which involves the use of the same case suffixes found in the adnominal and adverbial systems. Non-subject NPs are marked for a case category that is determined by the aspect-mood domain they are in. For example, if the verb has the past suffixal inflection, all non-subject nouns will then bear the modal ablative case (MABL).

birangkarra

long.time

bi-l-da

3-PL-NOM

mardala-tha

paint-ACT

dangka-walath-i,

man-a.lot-MLOC

ngimi-marra-y

night-UTIL-MLOC

birangkarra bi-l-da mardala-tha dangka-walath-i, ngimi-marra-y

long.time 3-PL-NOM paint-ACT man-a.lot-MLOC night-UTIL-MLOC

'They have been painting the men for a long time, getting ready for (the dance) tonight.' (Evans 1995: 109)[5]

The modal locative (MLOC) marks two constituents as being in the scope of the (unmarked) "instantiated" verb category, whereas the utilitive case (UTIL) expresses an expected use.

Case-stacking and concord

One of the functions of the modal cases proposed by Heath (2010) is that, such internal operation of case-stacking may mark the limits of the domain through the repetition of all nouns in the scopal domain of a clause-level inflectional category.[4] The modal cases is used to define the boundaries of clauses.

Case-stacking is closely relevant to the concept of concord, which has also been studied in other case-stacking languages such as Lardil and Australian languages in general. The term "concord" is defined as "the morphological realization, on multiple words dominated by a syntactic node n, of a morphosyntactic feature value associated with n."[7]

[DP[DP balarrinabawu dangkanabawu ABL] thungalu PROP]
white-‹ABL›-PROP man-‹ABL›-PROP thing-PROP
'having a white man's thing' (Round 2012:4.7)

In this example, all three words are dominated by the matrix DP node, and all three bear a proprietive case (PROP). The words within the subordinate DP [balarrinabawu dangkanabawu] ('white man') are dominated by both the matrix and the subordinate node, so both bear the inflectional ablative case (ABL) and proprietive case (PROP). This clearly illustrates the relationship of concord, where the sub-constituents of a syntactic node (i.e., matrix DP, in this example) carry the associating morphosyntactic features.

Syntactic structure in Kayardild case-stacking

The height of the syntactic nodes where features and cases attach to within the tree can vary by the relative positions of their domains. When a word carries more than one feature, its relative syntactic height generally corresponds to the linear order of its morphological realization.[7]

Lardil

[edit]

Lardil is an Australian language that allows case-stacking where nominals may surface with multiple case affixes. When new morphology is added to a nominal with a semantically uninterpretable case (e.g., accusative), the uninterpretable case is eliminated; however, when a semantically interpretable case is added (e.g., instrumental), the new morphology may be stacked outside the case morpheme. Richards (2013) argues that such uninterpretable cases are first assigned, then dropped.[8]

Ngada

I

derlde

break

marun-ngan-i

boy-GEN-ACC

wangalk-i.

boomerang-ACC

Ngada derlde marun-ngan-i wangalk-i.

I break boy-GEN-ACC boomerang-ACC

'I broke the boy's boomerang.' (Richards 2012: 20)

From this example, the possessor of the direct object is marked with the genitive suffix ‐ngan first, then with another suffix -i to indicate accusative. It is presumed that the genitive case is assigned by DP internal structures, i.e., the determiner itself; whereas the subsequent cases and features are assigned by higher heads, i.e., the verb, in the above example. This example illustrates the two different sources for the cases involved in stacking, as the genitive case morpheme and the accusative case morpheme are c-commanded and assigned by different constituents.

The order of case is also important in Lardil. In the following example (a), the direct object, wangalkuru [boomerang‐FUT-ACC], bears future case determined by tense concord within its own clause, and then followed by a subsequent accusative suffix determined by case concord with the subject maruni [boy‐ACC]. These affixes are arranged in this order, and the opposite order of affixation would yield an unattested form in (b).

(a)
Attested form

Ngada

I

kangka

say

marun-i

boy-ACC

were-thuru-Ø

throw-FUT-ACC

wangalk-uru-Ø.

boomerang-FUT-ACC

Ngada kangka marun-i were-thuru-Ø wangalk-uru-Ø.

I say boy-ACC throw-FUT-ACC boomerang-FUT-ACC

'I told the boy to throw the boomerang.' (Richards 2012: 21-22)

(b)
Unattested form

*wangalk-in-ku(r)

boomerang-ACC-FUT

*wangalk-in-ku(r)

boomerang-ACC-FUT

It is suggested that the affixation order is determined by the order of their assignment. In (a), the future case is assigned by the T of the embedded control clause, and the accusative suffix by the case concord with the subject. This results in the order of 'FUT-ACC' in the stacking of cases.

Korean

[edit]

Korean is a language that exhibits the patterns of case stacking. In general, Korean prohibits a nominal from bearing both the nominative and the accusative case at the same time.[9] However, the nominal can appear in either the dative case, the nominative case, or the accusative case, or in multiple cases where one is stacked on top of the other.[10]

철수에게가

Chelswu-eykey-ka

Chulsoo-DAT-NOM

돈이

ton-i

money-NOM

필요하다

phil-yoha-ta

need-IN

철수에게가 돈이 필요하다

Chelswu-eykey-ka ton-i phil-yoha-ta

Chulsoo-DAT-NOM money-NOM need-IN

'Chulsoo needs money." (Gerdts & Youn 2000: 1)

In this example, the nominative case morpheme -ka is optionally suffixed to the subject (i.e., "Chulsoo") in addition to the dative suffix -eykey. The nature and assignment of -ka in the context of case-stacking is an ongoing debate. In addition, the acceptance of case-stacking in Korean speakers may vary upon context. Some speakers may need to consider case stacking in particular contexts in order to accept it (e.g., spoken context).[11] However, judgment studies also show that Korean speakers judge double accusative ditransitives as unacceptable yet they are not completely avoided when completing sentences.[12] In fact, case stacking is difficult for speakers to accept in most situations unless a particle such as man 'only' or kkaci 'even' intervenes between the dative and nominative case markers,[13] such as the following example:

나에게만이

Na-eykey-man-i

I-DAT-only-NOM

뱀이

paym-i

snake-NOM

무섭다

mwusepta

fearful

나에게만이 뱀이 무섭다

Na-eykey-man-i paym-i mwusepta

I-DAT-only-NOM snake-NOM fearful

'Only I am afraid of snakes.' (J. Yoon 1996: 110)

Although case-stacking has been used in various inflectional suffixes in other languages, in Korean, it is ungrammatical to stack the negator before a predicate. This is different from the restrictions on multiple occurrences of the negative prefixes in Korean, which states that a predicate can have only one of these prefixes, resulting in ungrammaticality in forms such as *pul-pul-A, *pi-pi-A, etc. However, the short-form negation an(i) can precede a negative prefix such as pu(l)-, pi-, and mi-, but not before another short-form negation of an(i) itself. This results in attested forms such an(i) pul-kanungha- 'impossible', and an(i) pi-kwahakceki- 'unscientific', as well as unattested forms like *an(i) an(i) kanungha-, and *an(i) an(i) kwahakceki-.[14]

Sumerian

[edit]

In Sumerian, affixes are arranged in a specific order in the noun phrase, such that affixes may not necessarily be directly attached[clarification needed] to nouns. The phenomenon is known as double case marking or case displacement.[15]

dumu

son

tur

young

lugal

king

gal~gal

great

kalam.ak.ak.ene.ra

homeland-GEN-GEN-PL-DAT

dumu tur lugal gal~gal kalam.ak.ak.ene.ra

son young king great homeland-GEN-GEN-PL-DAT

'for the young sons of the great kings of the homeland' (Michalowski 2020)

In the above example, the genitive NP [kalam] ("homeland") has been assigned multiple markers in reference to the head noun "sons", namely, the genitive marker -ak, the plural -ene, and the dative case -ra. Sumerian nouns may be marked for case, therefore, possessive relationships (e.g., genitive), plurals, and case marking were expressed by suffixed morphemes that stacked onto one another within the noun phrase.[15]

Old Georgian

[edit]

A subject, for instance, would be marked with a subjective affix as well as a genitive affix. So, for example, in Old Georgian

perx-ni

foot-NOM.PL

k'ac-isa-ni

man-GEN-NOM.PL

perx-ni k'ac-isa-ni

foot-NOM.PL man-GEN-NOM.PL

'a man's feet'

the genitival noun phrase agrees in case (nominative) and number (plural) with the head noun.[16] However, while such a possessive construction is most frequently found in Suffixaufnahme, other nominal constructions may also show similar behavior. In Old Georgian, a postpositional phrase modifying a noun could take on that noun's case and number features:

"Ra

 

turpa

 

prinvelia!"

 

c'amoidzaxa

 

ert-ma

one-ERG

bavshv-ta-gan-ma

child-GEN.PL-from-ERG

"Ra turpa prinvelia!" c'amoidzaxa ert-ma bavshv-ta-gan-ma

{} {} {} {} one-ERG child-GEN.PL-from-ERG

"'What a wonderful bird!' exclaimed one of the children"

has the ergative (also called narrative) case -ma on ertma repeated in the modifying postpositional phrase, headed by -gan.

Kashmiri

[edit]

Case-stacking in Kashmiri mostly occurs to genitive construction. By adding the suffix -hund or -sund to the noun, they indicate the gender and number of their modifier. The genitive takes the case marker of the argument's function, such that the genitive is marked with the ablative case before a postposition.[17]

kor-en-hɨnd-is

girl[F]-OBJ.PL-GEN.F.PL-OBJ.SG

mukābal-as=manz

comparison[M]-OBJ.SG=in

kor-en-hɨnd-is mukābal-as=manz

girl[F]-OBJ.PL-GEN.F.PL-OBJ.SG comparison[M]-OBJ.SG=in

'in comparison to the girls' (Verbeke 2017: 11)

In the above example, the suffixes -en, -hind, and -is are stacked onto each other after the stem [kor] ("girl") to indicate the respective genitive and ablative cases involved.

Distribution of case-stacking

[edit]

Living languages

[edit]

Dead languages

[edit]

Ancient languages

[edit]

Significance and debates on case-stacking

[edit]

Theoretical issues in morphology and syntax

[edit]

Regarding the phenomenon of Suffixaufnahme, Frans Plank (1995)[1] has proposed several theoretical issues in morphology and syntax in his influential work as following:

  • Suffixaufnahme represents an oddity in case and agreement that challenges previous theories of both. Such oddity partly originates from parts of speech and inflection patterns that have been influenced by Suffixaufnahme. For example, case-agreeing words serving as nouns rather than adjectives, and the genitive case being involved as an inflection rather than derivational, etc. Suffixaufnahme also emphasizes more on case-agreeing inflected nouns than case-agreeing derived adjectives.
  • There are implications on grammatical relations since case agreement have been assumed to encode attribution, apposition, or predication.
  • Noun phrase constituency and the "depth" or "flatness" of syntax are seen as a core parameter of typological variation, which may be relevant to agglutinative versus flective morphology. The contribution of morphological typology to the linguistic environment can be conducive or hostile to Suffixaufnahme itself.

The nature of case marking

[edit]

Case for Korean case-stacking

[edit]

One of the studied phenomena is when the "nominative" particle -ka can be optionally suffixed to the subject in addition to the dative particle -eykey. However, there is an ongoing debate between the cases for and against case-stacking. Traditionally, it has been assumed that ka is a case morpheme independent of the inherent dative case and is used to license NPs in the structure.[13] It is claimed that the case of the dative subject itself is inherently insufficient to license its appearance in subject position, therefore, it must receive an additional case in its structure (e.g., the nominative case ka). By this, the nominative case itself can also be overtly realized.

This view contradicts the claim that nominative/accusative cases behave genuinely as case-markers. Instead, it suggests that case alternations and grammatical subjecthood do not require nominals to exhibit nominative stacking. However, nominative stacked nominals behave similarly like major subjects, where nominative stacking reflect their status as non-nominative major subjects.[11]

Case against Korean case-stacking

[edit]

Against the traditional approach, one of the hypotheses is made by Carson Schütze (2001)[13] states that: "Despite the initial plausibility, stacked case particles are not genuine case-markers. Even unstacked Nominative and Accusative case-markers are ambiguous between marking case and discourse functions like Topic and Focus." It is proposed that the stacked ka is not a reflection of case. Instead, it should be treated as a focus particle rather than a case morpheme. Moreover, NP subjects do not require additional morphological nominative case features to be licensed. In fact, the NP that is lexically marked as dative by its predicate is not even eligible for an additional structural case (e.g., nominative or accusative).

The evidence against traditional claims proposed by Schütze are as follows:

  1. Case-stacked sentences require a specific prosody to sound felicitous, e.g., an intonation phrase boundary is required after the subject. If ka were a nominative case marker, additional theories would be needed to account for this prosodic effect.
  2. ka-stacking is completely optional. However, when the case particle is present on the object, the nominative case on the subject of a transitive clause becomes obligatory.
  3. Stacked and unstacked ka can affect the distribution of subject honorification differently. Nominative subjects must trigger honorific agreement on the predicate but dative subjects cannot. If stacked ka were a nominative case, it should require honorific agreement, but it is actually disallowed.
  4. ka-stacking is not limited to subjects, but also occurs on locative, directional, and temporal adjuncts, etc., where structural nominative case is not assignable.
  5. Stacked and unstacked ka behave differently with respect to a particular kind of quantifier floating. In Korean, floated numeral quantifiers agree with their head noun in case. If stacked ka reflected morphological nominative case features, then one should allow a nominative quantifier to agree with a stacked ka subject.
  6. Stacking can be applied to nominative subjects and not only to the nominative case. The nominative markers can appear on the nominative subject in honorifics.

Evidence of ka as a focus marker

  1. Stacking can occur in wh-phrases.
  2. Stacking is possible on the answer to a subject wh-question.
  3. Stacking can occur in correction contexts.
  4. Stacking is compatible with overt focus markers such as 'only' or honorific dative markers.
  5. Korean as a multiple focus language allows a double focus reading.
  6. ka-stacking is obligatory on the complement to the negated copula anila.
  7. With an indefinite dative subject, it is ambiguous between existential and specific readings; but when ka is stacked on the subject, it can only be specific.

Competing models in case assignment

[edit]

One of more common models in case assignment is the Agree model, which states that structural case features are assigned to nominals by functional heads. Given a head that can assign case and a nominal that is c-commanded by the head, the case-marking associated with the head will be assigned to the nominal.[18] Another competing model is the Dependent model, which proposes that the case that a nominal receives is dependent on the presence of other nominals in a local domain. In an ergative language (i.e., downwards), the c-commanding nominal receives dependent case; in an accusative language (i.e., upwards), the c-commanded nominal receives the dependent case. Every nominal that is not assigned a dependent case will then bear an unmarked case (i.e., 'nominative' or 'absolutive'). However, within the nominal domain, the unmarked case is represented as the 'genitive' case.[18] In this model, the role of individual functional heads is less direct.

The Dependent model may be applied to Korean case-stacking. The direct object is c-commanded by the subject within vP phrase.

Mary-ka John-ul ttayliesse
Mary-NOM John-ACC hit
'Mary hit John.' (Levin 2017: 8)

In this example, neither the direct object John nor the subject Mary receives lexical case. The direct object John receives the dependent accusative case. However, the subject Mary receives an unmarked nominative case since it is not assigned a dependent case and its structural position does not satisfy the requirements on the realization of either the lexical or dependent case.[18]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Plank, Frans (1995). Double Case: Agreement by Suffixaufnahme. New York: Oxford University Press.
  2. ^ Brandner, Ellen; Zinsmeister, Heike (2003). New perspectives on case theory. Sanford, CA: CSLI publications. pp. 165–190. ISBN 1575863634.
  3. ^ Sadler, Louisa; Nordlinger, Rachel (2006). "Case stacking in realizational morphology" (PDF). Linguistics. 44 (3): 459–487. doi:10.1515/LING.2006.016. S2CID 143610461.
  4. ^ a b Heath, Jeffrey (2010). "Typology of clausal boundary marking devices". Linguistic Typology. 14: 127–151. doi:10.1515/lity.2010.004. S2CID 45203673.
  5. ^ a b c Evans, Nicholas (1995). A grammar of Kayardild. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  6. ^ Kracht, Marcus (2003). Against the Feature Bundle Theory of Case. CSLI Publications. pp. 165–190.
  7. ^ a b Round, Erich (2012). Kayardild Morphology and Syntax. Oxford University Press.
  8. ^ Richards, Norvin (2013). "Lardil "Case Stacking" and the Timing of Case Assignment". Syntax. 16: 42–76. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9612.2012.00169.x.
  9. ^ Gerdts, Donna B.; Youn, Cheong (1988). "Korean psych constructions: Advancement or retreat?". CLS. 24: 155–175.
  10. ^ Gerdts, Donna B.; Cheong, Youn (2000). "Case stacking and focus in Korean". CiteSeerX 10.1.1.459.5731.
  11. ^ a b Yoon, James (2004). "Non-nominative (Major) Subjects and Case Staking in Korean". Typological Studies in Language. 61: 265–314. doi:10.1075/tsl.61.15yoo. ISBN 978-90-272-2971-7.
  12. ^ Park, Sang-Hee (2021). "Perception-production asymmetry for Korean double accusative ditransitives". Linguistic Research. 38: 27–52.
  13. ^ a b c Schütze, Carson (2001). "On Korean "Case Stacking": The varied functions of the particles ka and lul". The Linguistic Review. 18 (3): 193–232. doi:10.1515/tlir.2001.001.
  14. ^ Chung, Inkie (2009). "Suppletive verbal morphology in Korean and the mechanism of vocabulary insertion". Journal of Linguistics. 45 (3): 533–567. doi:10.1017/S0022226709990028. S2CID 37638234.
  15. ^ a b Michalowski, Piotr (2020). Sumerian. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. pp. 83–105.
  16. ^ Kracht, Marcus (2002). "Suffixaufnahme". ms., FU Berlin.
  17. ^ Verbeke, Saartje (2017). Argument structure in Kashmiri. Brill.
  18. ^ a b c Levin, Theodore (2017). "Successive-cyclic case assignment: Korean nominative-nominative case-stacking". Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. 35 (2): 447–498. doi:10.1007/s11049-016-9342-z. S2CID 16782842.